Main Article Content

Authors

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) has been associated with deficits in executive functions, but no cognitive profile is clearly associated with the combined subtype (ADHD-C). The goals of this study were to compare the inhibitory control and the monitoring of students with and without ADHD-C, and to analyze the correlation between experimental tasks and scales for behavioral evaluation with the referred executive functions. The study had 68 participants, including an experimental group of 34 students with ADHD-C and a control group of 34 students reflecting typical development. The instruments for measurement were the behavioral scales BRIEF, BIS 11-c, and ADHD Rating Scale IV, and the experiments were based on Go/No-Go, Simon, and Victoria Stroop tasks. It was found that the experimental group with ADHD presented deficits in the executive functions evaluated, and that the behavioral scales and the experiments evaluated different levels of executive function.

Carlos Ramos, Universidad Internacional SEK del Ecuador

  • PSICÓLOGO INFANTIL Y PSICOREHABILITADOR - Universidad Central del Ecuador (2008)
  • MAGÍSTER EN NEUROPSICOLOGÍA CLÍNICA Y REHABILITACIÓN NEUROPSICOLÓGICA - Universidad Central del Ecuador (2011)
  • MAGÍSTER EN PEDAGOGÍA - Universidad Técnica Particular de Loja (2013)
  • DOCTOR EN PSICOLOGÍA (2015). Universidad de Concepción-Chile.
Ramos, C., & Pérez-Salas, C. (2017). Inhibitory Control and Monitoring in Child Population with ADHD. Avances En Psicología Latinoamericana, 35(1), 117–130. https://doi.org/10.12804/revistas.urosario.edu.co/apl/a.4195

Referencias

Alloway, T., Gathercole, S., Holmes, J., Place, M., Elliot, J., & Hilton, K. (2009). The diagnostic utility of behavioral checklist in identifying children with ADHD and children with working memory deficits . Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 40 (3), 353-366. doi: 10.1007/s10578-009-0131-3

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association .

American Psychiatric Association. (2014). Guía de consulta de los criterios diagnósticos del DSM-5. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing.

Arango, O., Puerta, I., & Pineda, D. (2008). Estructura Factorial de la Función Ejecutiva desde el Dominio Conductual. Revista Diversitas - Perspectivas en Psicología, 63-77.

Barkley, R. (1997). Behavioral Inhibition, Sustained Attention, and Executive Functions: Constructing a Unifying Theory of ADHD. Psichological Bulletin Vol. 121, No. 1, 65-94. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.65

Barkley, R. (2011). Avances en el diagnóstico y la subclasificación del trastorno por déficit de atención/hiperactividad: qué puede pasar en el futuro respecto al DSM-V. Revista de Neurología, 48 (2), 101-106.

Barkley, R., & Fischer, M. (2011). Predicting impairment in Major Life Activities and Occupational Functioning Hyperactive Children as Adults: Self-Reported Executive Function (EF) Deficits Versus EF Tests. Developmental Neuropsychology, 36 (2), 137-161. doi:10.1080/87565641.2010.549877

Bezdjian, S., Baker, L., Lozano, D., & Raine, A. (2009). Assessing inattention and impulsivity in children during the Go/NoGo task. Br J Dev Psychol, 27 (2), 365-383. doi:10.1348/026151008X314919

Bioulac, S., Lallemand, S., Rizzo, A., Philip, P., Fabrigoule, C., & Bouvard, M. (2012). Impact of time on task on ADHD patient´s perormances in a virtual classroom. European Journal of Paediatric Neurology, 16 (5), 514-521.

doi: 10.1016/j.ejpn.2012.01.006.

Brown, T. (2008). ADD/ADHD and impaired executive function in clinical practice. Current Psychiatry Reports, 10 (5), 407-411.

Chahin, N., Cosi, S., Lorenzo-Seva, U., & Vigil-Colet, A. (2010). Stablity of the factor structure of Barrat's Impulsivity Scales for children across cultures: A comparison of Spain and Colombia. Psicothema, 22 (4), 983-989.

Damasio, A. (1994). El error de Descartes. Santiago de Chile: Editorial Andrés Bello.

De la Barra, F., Vicente, B., Saldivia, S., & Melipillan, R. (2013). Epidemiology of ADHD in Chilean children and adolescents. ADHD Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorders, 5 (1), 1-8.

DuPaul, G., Power, T., Anastopoulos, A., Reid, R., Kara, M., & Ikeda, M. (1997). Teacher Ratings of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Symptoms: Factor Structure and Normative Data. Psychological Assessment, 9 (4), 436-444.

García, T., González-Pienda, J., Rodríguez, C., Álvarez, D., & Álvarez, L. (2014). Psychometric characteristics of the BRIEF scale for the assessment of executive functions in Spanish clinical population. Psicothema, 26 (1), 47-52.

Gioia, G., Isquith, P., Guy, S., & Kenworthy, L. (2000). Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function. Lutz, Florida: Psychological Assessment Resources.

Gioia, G., Isquith, P., Retzlaff, P., & Espy, K. (2002). Confirmatory Factor Analysis of the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) in a Clinical Sample. Child Neuropsychology, 8 (4), 249-257.

doi: 10.1076/chin.8.4.249.13513

Halperin, J., Trampush, J., Miller, C., Marks, D., & Newcorn, J. (2008). Neuropsychological outcome in adolescents/young adults with childhood ADHD: profiles of persisters, remitters and controls. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49 (9), 958-966.

Holmes, J., Gathercole, S., Place, M., Alloway, T., Elliott, J., & Hilton, K. (2010). The Diagnostic Utility of Executive Function Assessments in the Identification of ADHD in Children. Child and Adolescent Mental Health, Volume 15, 37-43. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-3588.2009.00536.x

Hughes, A., Wilson, F. T., & Emslie, H. (2013). Detecting executive deficits in children with ADHD or acquired brain injury using the Behavioural Assessment of Dysexecutive Sndrome (BADS). The Irish Journal of Spychology, 34 (1), 13-23.

Kunde, W., & Stocker, C. (2002). A Simon effect for stimulus-response duration. Quartely Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55, 581-592. doi:10.1080/02724980143000433

Lezak, M. (1982). The problem of Assesing Executive Functions. International Journal Psychology, 17, 281-297. doi: 10.1080/00207598208247445.

Luria, A. (1984). El cerebro en acción. Barcelona: Editorial Martínez Roca.

Mahone, E., Cirino, P., Cutting, L., Cerrone, P., Hagelthrorn, K., Hiemenz, J., & Denckla, M. (2002). Validity of the behavior rating inventory of executive function in children with ADHD and/or Tourette syndrome. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 17 (7), 643-662. doi:10.1016/S0887-6177(01)00168-8

Malek, A., Hekmati, I., Amiri, S., Pirzadeh, J., & Gholizadeh, H. (2013). The Standarization of Victoria Stroop Color-Word Test among Iranian Bilingual Adolescents. Archives of Iranian Medicine, 16 (7), 380-384.

doi: 013167/AIM.004

Mateo, V., & Vilaplana, A. (2007). Estrategias de identificación del alumno inatento e impulsivo desde el contexto escolar. Quaderns Digitals, 5, 13-28.

Miyake, A., Friedman, N., Emerson, M., Witzki, A., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex x ‘frontal Lobe’ tasks:a latent variable analysis. Cogn Psychol, 41, 49-100. doi:10.1006/cogp.1999.0734

Mueller, S., & Piper, B. (2014). The Psychology Experiment Building Language (PEBL) and PEBL Test Battery. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 222, 250-259. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2013.10.024.

Norman, D., & Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action: willed and automatic control of behavior. En R. Davidson, G. Schwartz, & D. Shapiro, Consciousness and self-regulation (págs. 1-18). New York: Editorial Plenumm Press.

Ramalho, J., García-Señorán, M., & González, S. (2011). Auto-instruções: estratégia de regulação atencional da THDA. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 24 (1), 180-185. doi:org/10.1590/S0102-79722011000100021

Roca, P., Presentación-Herrero, M., Miranda-Casas, A., Mulas, F., & Ortiz-Sánchez, P. (2014). El componente P300 como correlato neurofisiológico de la memoria de trabajo conductual en adolescentes con trastorno por déficit de atención con hiperactividad. Revista de Neurología, 58 (1), S51-S56.

Rodríguez, A., & Ros, S. (2004). Los trastornos puros. En S. Ros, M. Peris, & R. García, Imopulsividad. Primera edición (págs. 81-99). Barcelona: Ars Médica.

Romero-Ayuso, D., Maestú, F., González-Marqués, J., Romo-Barrientos, C., & Andrade, J. (2006). Disfunción ejecutiva en el trastorno por déficit de atención con hiperactividad en la infancia. Revista de Neurología 42 (5), 265-271.

Salum, G., Sergeant, E., Sonuga-Barke, E., Vandekerckhove, J., Gadelha, A., Pan, P., & Prhde, L. (2014). Specify of basic information proccesing and inhibitory control in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder . Psychological Medicine, 44, 617-631.doi: 10.1017/S0033291713000639

Sánchez-Carpintero, R., & Narbona, J. (2004). El sistema ejecutivo y las lesiones frontales en el niño. Revista de Neurología, 39 (2) , 188-191.

Servera, M. (2005). Modelo de autorregulación de Barkley aplicado al Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad: una revisión. Revista de Neurología, 40 (6), 358-368.

Servera, M., & Cardo, E. (2007). ADHD Rating Scale-IV en una muestra escolar española: datos normativos y consistencia interna para maestros, padres y madres. Revista de Neurología, 45 (7), 393-399.

Shallice, T., Marzocchi, G., Coser, S., DelSavio, M., Meuter, R., & Rumiati, R. (2002). Executive function profile of children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Developmental Neuropsychology, 21, 43-71. doi:10.1016/S0887-6177(03)00122-7

Skogli, E., Egeland, J., Norman, P., Tore, K., & Øie, M. (2014). Few differences in hot and cold executive functions in children and adolescents with combined and inattentive subtypes of ADHD. Child Neuropsychology: A Journal on Normal and Abnormal Development in Childhood and Adolescence, 20 (2), 162-181. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2012.753998.

Troyer, A., Leach, L., & Strauss, S. (2006). Aging and response inhibition: Normative data for the Victoria Stroop Test. Aging,Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 13, 20-35. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.11.005

Vaughn, A., Epstein, J., Rausch, J., Altaye, J., Newcorn, J., Hinshaw, S., & Wigal, T. (2011). Relation between outcomes on a continuous performance test and ADHD symptoms over time. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39, 853-864.

Vélez Van Meerbeke, A., Zamora, I., Guzmán, B., López, C., & Talero-Gutierrez, C. (2013). Evaluación de la función ejecutiva en una población escolar con síntomas de déficit de atención e hiperactividad. Neurología, 28 (6), 348-355. doi:10.1016/j.nrl.2012.06.011

Williams, J. (2008). Revising the declaration of Helsinki. World medical journal. 54 (4), 120-122.

Winstanley, C., Theobald, E., Dalley, J., Cardinal, R., & Robbins, T. (2006). Double dissociation between serotonergic and dopaminergic modulation of medial prefrontal and orbitofrontal cortex during a test of impulsive choice. Cereb Cortex, 16(1), 106-114. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhi088

Winther, E., Egeland, J., Norman, P., Tore, K., & Øie, M. (2014). Few differences in hot and cold executive functions in children and adolescents with combined and inattentive subtypes of ADHD. Child Neuropsychology: A Journal on Normal and Abnormal Development in Childhood and Adolescence, 20 (2), 162-181.

Downloads

Download data is not yet available.

Similar Articles

<< < 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 > >> 

You may also start an advanced similarity search for this article.