Control inhibitorio en perros domésticos:¿qué sabemos hasta ahora?
Barra lateral del artículo
Contenido principal del artículo
Descargas
Jesica Paola FAGNANI, Icivet-UNL CONICET
Becaria Doctoral CONICET.Gabriela Barrera, Grupo de Investigación del Comportamiento en Cánidos (ICOC) Instituto de Ciencias Veterinarias del Litoral, Consejo Nacional de InvestigacionesCientíficas y Técnicas (ICiVet Litoral, CONICET-UNL
Becaria Doctoral CONICET.Mariana Bentosela, Grupo de Investigación del Comportamiento en Cánidos (ICOC) Instituto de Investigaciones Médicas, Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas (IDIM; CONICET-UBA)
Doctora en PsicologíaAddessi, E., Paglieri, F. & Focaroli, V. (2011). The ecological rationality of delay tolerance: insights from capuchin monkeys. Cognition, 119, 142–147.
Amici, F., Aureli, F. & Call, J. (2008). Fission-fusion dynamics, behavioral flexibility, and inhibitory control in primates. Current Biology 18(18), 1415–1419.
Barrera, G., Elgier, A., Jakovcevic, A., Mustaca, A. & Bentosela, M. (2009). Problemas de comportamiento en los perros domésticos (canisfamiliaris): aportes de la psicología del aprendizaje. Revista de Psicología, 18, 123-146.
Bentosela, M. & Mustaca, A. (2007). Comunicación entre perros (Canisfamiliaris) y hombres: Un negocio conveniente. Revista Latinoamericana de Psicología, 39 (2), 375-387.
Beran, M. & Evans, T. (2006). Maintenance of delay of gratification by four chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): The effects of delayed reward visibility, experimenter presence, and extended delay intervals. Behavioral Processes, 73(3), 315–324.
Beran, M., Evans, T.,Paglieri, F., McIntyre, J., Addessi, E. & Hopkins, W. (2014). Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) can wait, when they choose to: A study with the hybrid delay task. Animal Cognition, 17, 197-205.
Boogert, N., Anderson, R., Peters, S., Searcy, W. & Nowicki, S. (2011). Song repertoire size in male song sparrows correlates with detour reaching, but not with other cognitive measures. Animal Behaviour 81(6), 1209–1216.
Braüer, J., Bös, M., Call, J. & Tomasello, M. (2013). Domestic dogs (Canisfamiliaris) coordinate their actions in a problem-solving task. Animal Cognition, 16, 273–285.
Bray, E., MacLean, E. & Hare, B. (2014). Context specificity of inhibitory control in dogs. Animal Cognition 17, 15–31. Doi: 10.1007/s10071-013-0633-z
Broos, N., Schmaal, L., Wiskerke, J., Kostelijk, L., Lam, T., Stoop, N. et al. (2012). The Relationship between Impulsive Choice and Impulsive Action: A Cross-Species Translational Study. PLoS ONE, 7 (5), e36781. Doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0036781
Brosnan, S. & Beran, M. (2009).Trading behavior between conspecifics in chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 123, 181–194. Doi:10.1037/a0015092
Eagle, T. & Robbins, T. (2003). Inhibitory control in rats performing a stop-signal reaction-time task: effects of lesions of the medial striatum and D-amphetamine. Behavioral Neuroscience, 117, 1302–17.
Evans, T. & Beran, M. (2007). Chimpanzees use self-distraction to cope with impulsivity. Biology Letter, 22,599–602. Doi:10.1098/rsbl. 2007.0399
Evans, T., Beran, M., Paglieri, F. & Addessi, E. (2012). Delaying gratification for food and tokens in capuchin monkeys (Cebusapella) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): when quantity is salient, symbolic stimuli do not improve performance, Animal Cognition, 15, 539–548. Doi: 10.1007/s10071-012-0482-1
Evenden, J. (1999). Varieties of impulsivity. Psychopharm, 146 (348), 361-375.
Frank, H. & Frank, M. (1983). Inhibition training in wolves anddogs. Behavioral Processes, 8, 363-377.
Fratkin, L., Sinn, D., Patall, E. & Gosling, S. (2013). Personality consistency in dogs: a meta-analysis. PLoS ONE, 8, e54907. Doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0054907
Freidin, E., Putrino, N., D’Orazio, M. & Bentosela, M. (2013). Dogs’ Eavesdropping from People’s Reactions in Third Party Interactions. PLoS ONE, 8, (11), e79198. Doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0079198
Jacobs, C., van Den Broeck, W.& Simoens, P. (2006). Increased volume and neuronal number of the basolateral nuclear group of the amygdaloid body in aggressive dogs. Behavioral Brain Research, 170, 119-125.
Jakovcevic, A. & Bentosela, M. (2009). Diferencias individuales en los perros domésticos (canis familiaris): revisión de las evaluaciones conductuales. Interdisciplinaria, 26, (1), 49-76
Kubinyi, E., Vas, J., Hejjas, K., Ronai, Z., Brúder, I., Turcsán, B. et al. (2012). Polymorphism in the Tyrosine Hydroxylase (TH) Gene Is Associated with Activity-Impulsivity in German Shepherd Dogs. PLoS ONE, 7 (1), e30271. Doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0030271
Leonardi, J., Vick, S. & Dufour, V. (2012). Waiting for more: the performance of domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) on exchange tasks. Animal Cognition, 15, 107–120, Doi: 10.1007/s10071-011-0437-y
Lipszyc, J. & Schachar, R. (2010).Inhibitory control and psychopathology: A meta-analysis of studies using the stop signal task. Journal of the International Neuropsychiatric Society, 16, 1064–1076. Doi: 10.1017/ S1355617710000895
MacLean, E., Hare, B., Nunna, C., Addessi, E., Amici, F., Andersone, R. et al. (2014). The evolution of self-control. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA, 111 (20), 1-9. Doi: 10.1073/pnas.1323533111
Mazur, J. (1987). An adjusting procedure for studying delayed reinforcement.EnCommons, M., et al. (Eds), Quantitative Analyses of Behavior: The Effect of Delay and of Intervening Events on Reinforcement Value (Vol. 5; pp. 55–73). New York: Lawrence Erlbaum
McGuire, J. & Kable, J. (2012). Decision makers calibrate behavioral persistence on the basis of time-interval experience. Cognition, 124, 216–226.
Miklósi, A., Topál, J.& Csányi,V. (2004). Comparative social cognition: what can dogs teach us. Animal Behavior, 67, 995-100.
Miller, H., Pattison, K., DeWall, C., Rayburn-Reeves, R. & Zentall, T. (2010). Self-Control Without a ''Self''?: Common Self-Control Processes in Humans and Dogs. Psychological Science, 21, 534. Doi: 10.1177/0956797610364968
Miller, H., DeWall, N., Pattison, K., Molet, M. & Zentall, T. (2012). Too dog tired to avoid danger: Self-control depletion in canines increases behavioral approach toward an aggressive threat. Psychological Bulletin Review, 19, 535–540. Doi: 10.3758/s13423-012-0231-0
Mischel, W.& Rodriguez, M. (1993).Psychological distance in self-imposed delay of gratification. EnCocking,R. &Renninger, K. (Eds.), The development and meaning of psychological distance. Hillsdale: Lawrence Earlbaum.
Mischel, W. (2014). The Marshmallow Test: Mastering Self-control. New York: Hachette Digital, Inc.
Ostojic, L. & Clayton, N. (2013). Behavioural coordination of dogs in a cooperative problem-solving task with a conspecific and a human partner. Animal Cognition, 17, 445-459. Doi: 10.1007/s10071-013-0676-1
Osvath, M. & Osvath, H. (2008). Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) and orangutan (Pongoabelii) forethought: self-control and pre-experience in the face of future tool use. Animal Cognition, 11, 661–674. Doi: 10.1007/s10071-008-0157-0
Paglieri, F., et al (2013). The hybrid delay task: Can capuchin monkeys (Cebusapella) sustain a delay after an initial choice to do so? Behavioral Processes, 94, 45-54.
Parrish, A., Perdue, B., Evans, T. & Beran, M. (2013). Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) transfer token repeatedly with a partner to accumulate rewards in a self-control task. Animal Cognition, 16 (4), 627-36. Doi: 10.1007/s10071-013-0599-x.
Pelé, M., Micheletta, J., Uhlrich, P., Thierry, B. & Dufour, V. (2011). Delay maintenance in Tonkean macaques and brown capuchin monkeys. International Journal of Primatology, 32, 149–166. Doi: 10.1007/s10764010-9446-y
Putrino, N., Jakovcevic, A., Carpintero, S., D´Orazio, M. & Bentosela, M. (2014). ¿Existen asociaciones entre sociabilidad, aprendizaje y comunicación entre perros y personas? Revista de Psicología, 23 (1), xx-xx.
Rachlin, H.& Green, L. (1972). Commitment, choice and self-control. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 17, 15–22.
Riemer, S., Mills, D. & Wright, H. (2013). Impulsive for life? The nature of long-term impulsivity in domestic dogs. Animal Cognition, 17 (3), 815-819. Doi: 10.1007/s10071-013-0701-4
Roberts, W. (2002). Are animals stuck in time? Psychological Bulletin, 128, 473–489.
Rosati, A., Stevens, J., Hare, B. & Hauser, M. (2007). The evolutionary origins of human patience: temporal preferences in chimpanzees, bonobos, and human adults. Current Biology, 17, 1663-1668.
Scott, J. & Fuller,J. (1965).Genetics and the Social Behaviour of the Dog. Chicago: University Chicago Press.
Scott, J., Shepard, J. &Werboff, J. (1967). Inhibitory training in dogs: Effects of age at training in basenjis and Shetland sheepdogs. Journal of Psychology, 66, 237-252.
Shifferman, E. (2009). Its own reward: lessons to be drawn from the reversed-reward contingency paradigm. Animal Cognition 12, 547-558. Doi:10.1007/s10071-009-0215-2
Soubrié, P. (1986). Reconciling the role of central serotonin neurons in human and animal behavior. Behavioral Brain Science, 9, 319-64.
Spady, T. & Ostrander, E. (2008). Canine Behavioral Genetics: Pointing Out the Phenotypes and Herding up the Genes. Journal of Human Genetics, 82, 10–18.
Stafford, K. (2007). The Welfare of Dogs. Dordrecht: Springer.
Stahl, C., Voss, A., Schmitz, F., Nuszbaum, M., Tüscher, O., Lieb, K. & Klauer, K. (2014). Behavioral Components of Impulsivity. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 143 (2), 850–886.
Stevens, J. & Stephens, D. (2009). The adaptive nature of impulsivity. En Madden,G. &Bickel, W. (Eds.), Impulsivity: The behavioral and neurological science of discounting (pp. 361-387). Washington: American Psychological Association.
Stevens, J. (2010). Intertemporal choice. EnBreed, M. &Moore, J. (Eds.), Encyclopedia of animal behavior (cap 6).Oxford: Academic Press.
Stevens, J., Rosati, A., Heilbronner, S. &Mühlhoff, N. (2011). Waiting for Grapes: Expectancy and Delayed Gratification in Bonobos. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 24, 99-111.
Tangney, J., Baumeister, R.& Boone, A. (2004). High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades and interpersonal success. Journal of Personality, 72, 271–324.
Topál, J., Gergely, G., Erdohegyi, A., Csibra, G. &Miklosi, A. (2009).Differential Sensitivity to Human Communication in Dogs, Wolves, and Human Infants. Science, 325 (5945), 1269-1272. Doi: 10.1126/science.1176960
Udell, M. & Wynne, C. (2010). Ontogeny and phylogeny: both are essential to human-sensitive behavior in the genus Canis. Animal Behavior, 79, e9-e14.
Van den Bergh, F., Spronk, M., Ferreira, L., Bloemarts, E., Groenink, L., Olivier, B. et al. (2006). Relationship of delay aversion and response inhibition to extinction learning, aggression, and sexual behaviour. Behavioral Brain Research, 175, 75–81.
Vas, J., Topál, J., Pách, E. & Miklósi, A. (2007). Measuring attention deficit and activity in dogs: A new application and validation of a human ADHD questionnaire. Applied Animal Behavioral Science, 103, 105-117.
Vick, S., Bovet, D. & Anderson, J. (2009). How do African grey parrots (Psittacuserithacus) perform on a delay of gratificationtask? Animal Cognition, 13, 351-358. Doi: 10.1007/s10071-009-0284-2
Vlamings, P. (2006). Inhibition in great apes: Performance on a detour-reaching and reversed contingency task. Tesis de Maestría. Netherlands: University of Maastricht.
Wan, M., Hejjas, K., Ronai, Z., Elek, Z., Sasvari-Szekely, M., Champagne, F. et al. (2013). DRD4 and TH gene polymorphisms are associated with activity, impulsivity and inattention in Siberian Husky dogs. Animal Genetics, 44, 717–727. Doi: 10.1111/age.12058
Wright, H., Mills, D. & Pollux, P. (2011). Development and validation of a psychometric tool for assessing impulsivity in the domestic dog (Canisfamiliaris). International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 24, 210–25.
Wright, H., Mills, D. & Pollux, P. (2012).Behavioural and physiological correlates of impulsivity in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris). Physiology & Behavior, 105, 676–682.
Wright, L., Lipszyc, J., Dupuis, A., Thayapararajah, S. &Schachar, R. (2014). Response inhibition and psychopathology: A meta-analysis of go/no-go task performance. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 123(2), 429-439. Doi: 10.1037/a0036295
Zaichenko, M. & Merzhanova, G. (2011). Studies of impulsivity in rats in conditions of choice between food reinforcements of different value. Neuroscience and Behavioral Physiology, 41, 445-451.