Contenido principal del artículo

Daniela Eva Jauck
Olga Alicia Peralta

Los dispositivos digitales están cada vez más presentes en la vida cotidiana y han comenzado a utilizarse en la comunicación y el aprendizaje a edades muy tempranas. Aunque padres y educadores suponen que los pequeños comprenden la función simbólica de sus imágenes, la incipiente pero creciente investigación en el área no ha arrojado resultados claros al respecto. El objetivo del presente estudio fue indagar si los niños pequeños comprenden que las imágenes provistas por una tablet representan acciones que pueden ocurrir en la realidad. Presentamos a niños de 24, 30 y 36 meses de edad imágenes de acciones cotidianas en secuencias que no mostraban el resultado de la acción. Luego, los niños debían escoger un objeto real que ostentaba el resultado de la acción. Encontramos que a los 30 y 36 meses, pero no a los 24, los niños comprenden que las imágenes representan una acción que puede ocurrir en la realidad. Los resultados se discuten en torno a la comprensión simbólica de imágenes y a la comprensión de acciones intencionales cuyo fin los niños deben imaginar.

Descargas

Los datos de descargas todavía no están disponibles.
Jauck, D. E., & Peralta, O. A. (2017). La comprensión simbólica de imágenes digitales que representan acciones dirigidas hacia un fin. Avances En Psicología Latinoamericana , 35(3), 601-614. https://doi.org/10.12804/revistas.urosario.edu.co/apl/a.5070

Daniela Eva Jauck, Instituto Rosario de Investigaciones en Ciencias de la Educación (IRICE-CONICET) Universidad Abierta Interamericana- Facultad de Psicología

Becaria Doctoral del Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas -CONICET, Argentina

Olga Alicia Peralta, Instituto Rosario de Investigaciones en Ciencias de la Educación (IRICE-CONICET) Universidad Abierta Interamericana- Facultad de Psicología

Investigadora Principal del Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas -CONICET, Argentina

Profesora Titular de Psicología Educacional- Universidad Abierta Interamericana

Anderson, D.R. & Pempek, T.A. (2005). Television and very young children. The American Behavioral Scientist, 48(5), 505-522. DOI: 10.1177/0002764204271506

Baldwin, D. A. (2000). Interpersonal understanding fuels knowledge acquisition. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 40–45. DOI: 10.1111/1467-8721.00057

Baldwin, D., Andersson, A., Saffran, J., & Meyer, M. (2008). Segmenting dynamic human action via statistical structure. Cognition, 106(3), 1382-1407. DOI:10.1016/j.cognition.2007.07.005

Balwin, D.A., & Mose, J. A. (2001). Links between Social understanding and early Word learning: Challenges to current accounts. Social Development, 10, 311-329. DOI: 10.1111/1467-9507.00168

Baldwin, D., Baird, J., Saylor, M., & Clark, A. (2001). Infants parse dynamic human action. Child Development, 72, 708-717. DOI: 10.1111/1467-8624.00310

Barr, R. (2013). Memory constraints on infant learning from picture books, television, and touchscreens. Child Development Perspectives, 7 (4), 205-210. DOI: 10.1111/cdep.12041

Barr, R. (2008). Attention and learning from media during infancy and early childhood. In S. L. Calvert & B. Wilson, The Handbook of Children, Media and Development (pp. 144-165). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.

Barresi, J., & Moore, C. (1996). Intentional relations and social understanding. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 19(1), 107-121 DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00041881

Behne, T., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2005). One-year-olds comprehend the communicative intentions behind gestures in a hiding game. Developmental Science, 8, 492–499. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00440.x

Brune, C. W., & Woodward, A. L. (2007). Social cognition and social responsiveness in 10-month-old infants. Journal of Cognition and Development, 8(2), 138-158. DOI: 10.1080/15248370701202331

Carpenter, M., Akhtar, N., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Fourteen through 18-month-old infants differentially imitate intentional and accidental actions. Infant Behavior and Development, 21, 315–330. DOI: 10.1016/S0163 6383(98)90009 1

Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2009). Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 148-153. DOI:10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.005

DeLoache, J. S (1987). Rapid change in the symbolic functioning of very young children. Science, 238(4833), 1556-1557. DOI: 10.1126/ science.2446392

DeLoache, J. S. (2004). Becoming symbol-minded. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 66-70. DOI:10.1016/j.tics.2003.12.004

DeLoache, J. S., & Burns, N. (1994). Early understanding of the representational function of pictures. Cognition, 52, 83-110. DOI: 10.1016/0010-0277(94)90063-9

Dennett, D., 1987. The Intentional Stance. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Dirks, J., & Gibson, E. (1977). Infants' perception of similarity between live people and their photographs. Child Development, 124-130. DOI: 10.2307/1128890

Falck-Ytter, T., Gredebäck, G., & Von Hofsten, C. (2006). Infants predict other people’s action goals. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 878-879. DOI: 10.1038/nn1729

Flanagan J. R., & Johansson, R. S. (2003) Action plans used in action observation. Nature, 424:769–77. DOI: 10.1038/nature01861

Gergely, G. & Csibra, G. (2003). Teleological reasoning in infancy: The naive theory of rational action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 287-292. DOI: 10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00128-1

Gerson, S., & Woodward, A. (2013). The goal trumps the means: Highlighting goals is more beneficial than highlighting means in means-end training. Infancy, 18(2), 289-302. DOI: 10.111/j.1532-7078.2012.00112.x

Gerson, S.A., & Woodward, A.L. (2014). Labels facilitate infants’ comparision of action goals. Journal of Cognition and Development, 15(2), 197-212. DOI:10.1080/15248372.2013.777842

Gerson, S., & Woodward, A. (2014). Learning from their own actions: The unique effect of producing actions on infants’ action understanding. Child Development, 85(1), 264-277. DOI: 10.1111/cdev.12115

Gerson, S., & Woodward, A. (2014). The joint role of trained, untrained, and observed actions at the origins of goal recognition. Infant Behavior and Development, 37(1), 94-104. DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2013.12.013

Hamlin, J. K, Hallinan, E. V, & Woodward, A. L. (2008). Do as I do: 7-month-old infants selectively reproduce others' goals. Developmental Science, 11(4):487-94. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00694.x

Harris, P.L., Kavanaugh, R.D. & Dowson, L. (1997). The depiction of imaginary transformation: Early comprehension of a symbolic function.

Cognitive Development, 12, 1-19. DOI: 10.1016/S0885-2014(97)90028-9

Ittelson, W. H. (1996). Visual perception of markings. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 3(2), 171-187. DOI: 10.3758/BF03212416

Jovanovic, B., Király, I., Elsner, B., Gergely, G., Prinz, W, & Aschersleben, G. (2007). The role of effects for infants’ perception of action goals. Psychologia, 50, 273-290.

Kellman P. J., & Spelke E. S. (1983). Perception of partly occluded objects in infancy. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 483-524. DOI:10.1016/0010-0285(83)90017-8

Kirkorian, H.L., & Pempek, T. (2013). Toddlers and touch screens: Potential for early learning? Zero to Three, 33, 32-37.

Krcmar, M. (2010). Assessing the research on media, cognitive development, and infants: Can infants really learn from television and videos?. Journal of Children and Media, 4(2), 119-134. DOI: 10.1080/17482791003629586

Peralta, O. A., & Salsa, A. M. (2011). Instrucción y desarrollo en la comprensión temprana de fotografías como objetos simbólicos. Anales de Psicología, 27 (1), 118-125.

Leslie, A. M. (1995). A theory of agency. En D. Sperber, D. Premack y A. J., Premack (Eds.), Causal Cognition: A Multidisciplinary Debate (pp. 121-141). Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press.

Meltzoff, A. N. (1995). Understanding the intentions of others: Re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Developmental Psychology, 31, 838–850. DOI: 10.1037/0012-1649.31.5.838

Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1997). Explaining facial imitation: A theoretical model. Early Development and Parenting, 6, 179–192. DOI: 10.1002/(SICI)1099-0917(199709/12)6:3/4<179::AID-EDP157>3.0.CO;2-R

Meltzoff, A. N. (1999). Origins of theory of mind, cognition and communication. Journal of Communication Disorders, 32(4), 251-269. doi:10.1016/S0021-9924(99)00009-X

Moore, R., Liebal, K., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Three-year-olds understand communicative intentions without language, gestures, or gaze. Interaction Studies, 14(1), 62-80. DOI:10.1075/is.14.1.05moo.

Nielsen, M., Simcock, G., & Jenkins, L. (2008). The effect of social engagement on 24‐month‐olds’ imitation from live and televised models. Developmental Science, 11(5), 722-731. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00722.x

Gerson, S. A., & Woodward, A. L. (2013). The goal trumps the means: Highlighting goals is more beneficial than highlighting means in means‐end training. Infancy, 18(2), 289-302. DOI: 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2012.00112.x

Onishi K. H., & Baillargeon, R. (2005). Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs?, Science. 308 (5719):255-8. DOI: 10.1126/science.1107621

Premack, D & Premack, A. J. (1995). Origins of human social competence. In M.S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The Cognitive Neurosciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Saylor, M. M., Baldwin, D. A., Baird, J. A., & LaBounty, J. (2007). Infants’ on-line segmentation of dynamic human action. Journal of Cognition and Development, 8(1), 113-128. DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.07.005

Richert, R.A., Robb, M.B., & Smith, E.I. (2011). Media as social partners: The social nature of young children´s learning from screen media. Child Development, 82 (1), 82-95. DOI:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01542.x

Rose, A. (1997). Infants’ transfer of response between two-dimensional and three-dimensional stimuli. Child Development, 48, 1086-1091. DOI:10.2307/1128366

Rodríguez Sánchez, A. (2012). Los Niños Menores de Tres años y la Televisión: Perspectivas de Investigación y Debate. Ministerio de Cultura de Colombia. Dirección de Comunicaciones. Bogotá.

Roseberry, S. Hirsh-Pasek K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2014) Skype me! Socially contingent interactions help toddlers learn language. Child Development, 85(3):956-70. DOI: 10.1111/cdev.12166

Schmitt, K. L., & Anderson, D. R. (2002). Television and reality: Toddlers’ use of visual information from video to guide behavior. Media Psychology, 4, 51-76. DOI: 10.1207/S1532785XMEP0401_03

Salsa, A. M., & Peralta, O. A. (2007). Routes to symbolization: Intentionality and correspondence in early understanding of pictures. Journal of Cognition and Development, 8 (1), 79-92. doi: 10.1207/s15327647jcd0801_4

Sigel, I. E. (1978). The development of pictorial comprehension. En B. S. Randhawa & W. E. Coffman (Eds.), Visual Learning, Thinking and Communication (pp. 93-111). Nueva York: Academic Press.

Sommerville, J. A., Woodward, A. L., & Needham, A. (2005). Action experience alters 3-month-old infants’ perception of others’ actions. Cognition, 96, B1-B11. DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2004.07.004

Spelke, E. S., Phillips, A., & Woodward, A. L. (1995). Infants' knowledge of object motion and human action. En: Causal Cognition: A Multidisciplinary Debate. Symposia of the Fyssen Foundation., (pp. 44-78). New York, NY, US: Clarendon Press/Oxford University Press.

Tare, M., Chiong, C., Ganea, P., & DeLoache, J. S. (2010). Less is more: How manipulative features affect children's learning from picture books. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 31, 395-400. DOI: 10.1016/j.appdev.2010.06.005

Tomasello, M. (1999). The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.

Troseth, G. L., & DeLoache, J.S., (1998). The medium can obscure the message: Young children´s understanding of video. Child Development, 69, 950-965. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.tb06153.x

Troseth, G. L. (2003). TV guide: two-year-old children learn to use video as a source of information. Developmental Psychology, 39(1), 140. 10.1037/0012-1649.39.1.140

Troseth, G. L., Saylor, M. M., & Archer, A. H. (2006). Young children's use of video as a source of socially relevant information. Child Development, 77(3), 786-799. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00903.x

Woodward, A. L. (1998). Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor’s reach. Cognition, 69(1), 1-34. DOI: 10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00058-4

Walsh, E. (2005). Los Niños Pequeños y los Aparatos Electrónicos. Berkely, California: Childcare Health Program.

Woodward, A.L. (2003). Infants’ developing understanding of the link between looker and object. Developmental Science, 6, 297–311. DOI:10.1111/1467-7687.00286

Woodward, A. L. (2009). Infants’ grasp of others’ intentions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 53-57. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01605.x

Woodward, A. L. (2013). Infant foundations of intentional understanding. En: M. R. Banaji & S. A. Gelman (Eds.). Navigating the Social World: A Developmental Perspective (pp.75-80). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Zack, E., Barr, R., Gerhardstein, P., Dickerson, K., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2009). Infant imitation from television using novel touch-screen technology. British Journal of Development Psychology, 27(1), 13-26. DOI: 10.1348/026151008X334700

Christakis, D. A., & Zimmerman, F. (2009). Young children and media. Limitations of current knowledge and future directions for research. American Behavioral Scientist 52 (8), 1177-1185. DOI: 10.1177/0002764209331540

Detalles del artículo